In a recent referee report I argued something I have argued in several reports before: if the effect of interest in a regression is an interaction, the control variables addressing possible confounds should be interactions as well. In this post I explain that argument using as a working example a 2011 QJE paper (.pdf) that…
Category: Unexpectedly Difficult Statistical Concepts
[79] Experimentation Aversion: Reconciling the Evidence
A PNAS paper (.pdf) proposed that people object "to experiments that compare two unobjectionable policies" (their title). In our own work (.pdf), we arrive at the opposite conclusion: people "don't dislike a corporate experiment more than they dislike its worst condition" (our title). In a forthcoming PNAS letter, we identified a problem with the statistical…
[71] The (Surprising?) Shape of the File Drawer
Let's start with a question so familiar that you will have answered it before the sentence is even completed: How many studies will a researcher need to run before finding a significant (p<.05) result? (If she is studying a non-existent effect and if she is not p-hacking.) Depending on your sophistication, wariness about being asked…
[70] How Many Studies Have Not Been Run? Why We Still Think the Average Effect Does Not Exist
We have argued that, for most effects, it is impossible to identify the average effect (datacolada.org/33). The argument is subtle (but not statistical), and given the number of well-informed people who seem to disagree, perhaps we are simply wrong. This is my effort to explain why we think identifying the average effect is so hard….
[57] Interactions in Logit Regressions: Why Positive May Mean Negative
Of all economics papers published this century, the 10th most cited appeared in Economics Letters , a journal with an impact factor of 0.5. It makes an inconvenient and counterintuitive point: the sign of the estimate (b̂) of an interaction in a logit/probit regression, need not correspond to the sign of its effect on the…
[50] Teenagers in Bikinis: Interpreting Police-Shooting Data
The New York Times, on Monday, showcased (.htm) an NBER working paper (.pdf) that proposed that "blacks are 23.8 percent less likely to be shot at by police relative to whites." (p.22) The paper involved a monumental data collection effort to address an important societal question. The analyses are rigorous, clever and transparently reported. Nevertheless, I do…
[46] Controlling the Weather
Behavioral scientists have put forth evidence that the weather affects all sorts of things, including the stock market, restaurant tips, car purchases, product returns, art prices, and college admissions. It is not easy to properly study the effects of weather on human behavior. This is because weather is (obviously) seasonal, as is much of what…
[42] Accepting the Null: Where to Draw the Line?
We typically ask if an effect exists. But sometimes we want to ask if it does not. For example, how many of the "failed" replications in the recent reproducibility project published in Science (.pdf) suggest the absence of an effect? Data have noise, so we can never say 'the effect is exactly zero.' We can…
[41] Falsely Reassuring: Analyses of ALL p-values
It is a neat idea. Get a ton of papers. Extract all p-values. Examine the prevalence of p-hacking by assessing if there are too many p-values near p=.05. Economists have done it [SSRN], as have psychologists [.html], and biologists [.html]. These charts with distributions of p-values come from those papers: The dotted circles highlight the excess of…
[39] Power Naps: When do Within-Subject Comparisons Help vs Hurt (yes, hurt) Power?
A recent Science-paper (.pdf) used a total sample size of N=40 to arrive at the conclusion that implicit racial and gender stereotypes can be reduced while napping. N=40 is a small sample for a between-subject experiment. One needs N=92 to reliably detect that men are heavier than women (SSRN). The study, however, was within-subject, for instance, its dependent…